IV. Does toxoplasmosis account for I’m weird?

What did I make of the results of that first study, of the answers of the students to those ten questions I put forth initially – could some of the characteristics not compatible with my personality type be somehow explained by an infection by Toxoplasma gondii? The first couple of years my endeavor was not successful (an understatement, to be sure, since I know many colleagues who’d use a pithier and also more fitting expression in relation to my trials). As I already mentioned, some of the questions correlated with a Toxoplasma infection to a certain extent, but the strength of the correlation wasn’t particularly striking. One of those questions even had an opposite correlation to that which I had originally predicted. The main reason (or perhaps the main excuse?) for the many years I set aside the results of this study, was my realization that it would be even more difficult to publish these results than to publish the results indicating changes in Cattell’s Personality Factors. The results I acquired as by-products of the original study, while surprising, were at least obtained using a standard scientific method. Cattell’s Questionnaire is a published psychological tool of long-standing; psychologists presume the meaning of the individual factors measured by the questionnaire, or at least agree on the existence of these factors. There was therefore a possibility that if I proved that a biological factor (specifically an infection by Toxoplasma) influences some of these psychological factors, I would sooner or later succeed in publishing my findings. But if the observed changes were related to a hitherto undescribed psychological factor, whose existence I deduced purely from introspection (through a self-observation), then the chance of publishing my results was substantially lower. Mathematically speaking, the probability of publishing a study which investigates an unusual phenomenon using hitherto undescribed factors ascertained by way of nonstandard methods approaches a limit of zero (see also Box 23 Why to use established scientific methods).

Box 23 Why to use established scientific methods

The answer is quite simple – because only by doing so is it possible to compare and therefore interconnect results obtained in different workplaces. Isolated results often don’t mean much; they achieve their true value only when related to other results from different scientists, often from studies dealing with entirely unrelated problems. For this reason, wherever at all possible, it’s best to use standard and preferably widely utilized methods. Scientists trained in former communist Czechoslovakia are often masters in the art of inventing new methods. Necessity indeed is the mother of invention – established methods were not available to them; they could not buy the appropriate standard chemicals nor tools, since funding for obtaining Western products was limited; and when funding happened to be sufficient, purchases from the outside had to be planned many years in advance, so that even if the long-desired instrument or chemical finally arrived, one had often forgotten, what he had needed it for. Under the communist regime the ability to improvise was the basic prerequisite for successful scientific practice, but today it is something of a two-edged sword. On one hand it allows the discovery of an oftentimes better, cheaper, faster or more effective method than that which is being standardly employed in a given field, but, on the other hand, results obtained through a nonstandard method are difficult to compare with results obtained in other workplaces (and of course also more difficult to publish in scientific journals). Newly developed methods often harbor unexpected obstacles. During my postdoctoral studies, for example, I tried to determine whether double-stranded RNA (dsRNA) present in Trichomonas cells floats freely in the cytoplasm or whether it is contained within virions. The standard method for isolating virions is separation of the cellular homogenate using the cesium chloride density-gradient ultracentrifugation. It is a time consuming method, the centrifugation itself spanning many hours; furthermore, cesium chloride is expensive, as is operating and maintaining the ultracentrifuge. I, however, discovered that I could obtain virions from the homogenate in less than an hour by precipitating them using the right concentration of cheap ammonium sulfate (3). In addition, I discovered that when I isolated nucleic acids in the presence of high concentrations of ammonium sulfate using acidic phenol instead of neutralized phenol (so I reached into the fridge and grabbed the wrong bottle, nobody’s perfect) all the DNA and single-stranded RNA disappeared – pure dsRNA remained in the resulting sample. The problem was that the electron microscopy showed these virions to be empty, devoid of any nucleic acids (fig. 16). I conceived daring hypotheses to explain this state of affairs, and tentatively allowed even for the possibility that the presence of virions might have nothing to do with the presence of dsRNA in Trichonomas cells. The reality was much more prosaic. During the nonstandard method of isolating virions by precipitation using ammonium sulfate, all the dsRNA escapes. Had I used the classical ultracentrifugation method, I would have saved myself a lot trouble.

 

Fig. 16 Viral particles observed by an electron microscope in the cellular homogenate of Trichomonas. When viral particles are precipitated by adding ammonium sulfate, all the nucleic acid molecules escape; therefore, the particles appear to be hollow. In reality, native particles contain double-stranded ribonucleic acid molecules. Photo by Jitka Štokrová.

               So I returned to those results only after fifteen years – and then actually just by chance. In the presence of my erstwhile PhD student Jitka Lindová, née Hanušová, I mentioned the beginning study of the effect of toxoplasmosis on the human psyche, and I admitted that at the onset of the study I observed that I let myself get duped by craftsmen. She didn’t want to believe me, for even then my suspicious nature was widely-known – some of my colleagues, for example, claimed that while other people may see around a corner, my perception extends past two or three. That in and of itself is not so surprising – as a toxo-positive person I have a high Cattel’s factor Protension (L, suspiciousness, vigilance), and as a scientist skepticism is part of my job description. I explained to her that it wasn’t just unbacked prattle on my part; I had stowed away tons of data that could be used to test my theory. And, if I remembered correctly, the results of the preliminary analyses certainly weren’t uninteresting – in particular, some of the questions I had used to test my hypothesis seemed to correlate at least weakly with an infection by Toxoplasma.

         At the time we were just beginning a new study and preparing to collect data from fresh 300-400 students. The main goal of the new project was to study people’s behavior through the method of experimental games. It was clear, however, that this main trial would have to be supplemented with other tests, so that our test subjects would not sit idle during the course of the trial (a terrible prospect, to be sure). It was clear that we’d again give them some psychological questionnaires, and it occurred to me that it would be quite interesting to try to once more look at the results of the original toxo-questionnaire – the questionnaire of ten questions targeting behavior that could heighten the risk of capture by a felid – select those couple of questions, which might really be linked with a Toxoplasma infection, and present those questions to a new sample of students.

             So over a weekend I looked up on the computer the old data obtained in the original study, and now, 15 years later, I analyzed them once more. The result was astonishing. It turned out not that more than just the answers to one or two questions were loosely linked with an infection by Toxoplasma. In reality almost half the questions were Toxoplasma-related. The correlation (statistical association) with toxoplasmosis was significant, sometimes even very significant. For some cases, the probability that the difference between the average answer of a toxo-positive person and that of a toxo-negative person could have been but a turn of fate was less than 1 per mille. Why had I not seen this 15 years ago, when I had that data at my disposal, and when I first analyzed it? Because then I was not yet particularly knowledgeable in statistics, and in evaluating the data I used an utterly unsuited method.

               I posed my questions to the students in the form of direct statements (made up of many partial assertions), and the test subjects were to choose the most fitting of three given answers. One statement, for example, read as follows: “Unexpected personal conflict with a brazen, rude, crude or mean person generally distresses me; in such a situation I am often unable to effectively defend myself. When a craftsman asks me for money, I usually give it to him, even when I know that he doesn’t deserve it. When I notice that a salesman is cheating me, usually I let it be.” Students were to answer 1, 2 or 3. One meant I agree, two I’m not sure, and three I disagree. I originally compared the answers of infected and uninfected people using the method called Student’s t-test. This method is decidedly not suitable for testing a variable that reaches only 3 values (1, 2, 3) – in other words a so-called ordinal type of variable (see Box 24 Types of variables from the perspective of statistics).

 

Box 24 Types of variables from the perspective of statistics

In “normal, everyday life” we differentiate between quantitative variables (temperature, body height) and qualitative variables (nationality, gender). But in statistics it makes more sense to classify variables as continuous and discrete. Continuous variables, such as the above-mentioned temperature and body height, are free to take on any value (but usually only within a certain range of accepted values). Discrete variables encompass two types of data. The first type are ordinal variables, which can be ordered from lowest to highest, though the individual values don’t have to be equally spaced. One example of an ordinal variable is the level of education (primary, secondary, high); another is the degree of agreement with the statement in the questionnaire expressed on a given scale (strongly agree, mostly agree, not sure, mostly disagree, strongly disagree). The other type of discrete variables are categorical variables – these cannot be ordered by size. Among these are citizenship, the type of fertilizer or medicine used, etc. A special type of categorical variables are binary variables – these can take on only two values, like infected by Toxoplasma/uninfected by Toxoplasma, had a traffic accident/didn’t have a traffic accident, male/female. Based on the type of variable we must select the right type of statistical test. If, for example, we’re comparing the height of men and women, we’ll most likely use Student’s t-test; if it’s the education level of men and women, we’ll use some nonparametric test, like the Mann-Whitney U-test.

 

During the new evaluation I was already well aware of this, so in the new analysis I used a nonparametric test, specifically the Kendall correlation, which, like the more common Spearman’s (rank correlation) test, fits that type of data (see Box 25 Parametric and nonparametric tests).

Using the new method, I discovered that the original ten questions had been very well selected, and that the relationship between the answers to these questions and toxoplasmosis was much stronger than in the case of Catell’s factors, which I had focused my studies on and because of which I had actually set aside these fascinating results for 15 years. But again it turned out that a more evident association could be observed in men than in women. This might have been due to not only the fact that we had more males in our group, but also that a man, on the basis of introspection, came up with the questions. The strongest correlation with toxoplasmosis was held by the statement: “My instinctive (reflexive) behavior in situations of imminent personal danger is rather disproportionately slow or passive. In situations when other people would likely be frightened and, without looking around or deliberating, jump back, I react more slowly or belatedly. When a car surges towards me or when a honking sounds behind me, there is a greater chance that I won’t

 

Box 25 Parametric and nonparametric tests

In practice, statistical tests are divided into parametric and nonparametric tests. Parametric tests, when used for the right type of data, are said to have more statistical power (that is, when they are used there exists a greater probability of a justified rejection of a null hypothesis) than corresponding nonparametric tests (see Box 21 Analysis of the power of a statistical test). This is understandable, since a parametric test uses a greater part of the information contained in the data: for example, it directly compares the body height of individuals in a group, whereas a nonparametric test uses but a small part of the information, most often the information about the order of the individuals according to their arrangement from tallest to shortest. In practice, the difference in the power of tests doesn’t tend to be great, usually under 10%. Parametric tests, however, are intended for data with a particular type of distribution, generally for continuous variables with a normal distribution. If our data has a different type of distribution – for example, if we’re dealing with ordinal data – a parametric test may yield a false result. In such a case it’s necessary to use a nonparametric test, or, where possible, a more powerful exact test, or at least a randomization test (see Box 83 Randomization tests).

If we’re comparing, for example, the average value of continuous data (like average height) between two groups (males and females), we’ll most likely use a parametric t-test; for multiple groups (Czechs, Germans, Chinese, English, Japanese), we’ll use the ANOVA (an analysis of variance). If we’re comparing paired continuous data (such as the body mass of the same mice before and after infection, see Box 67 What is the difference between the paired and unpaired t-tests, and why is the paired one better?), we’ll use a paired t-test. If we’re studying the effect of a certain factor taking on two or more levels (gender or ethnicity) and simultaneously the effect of another continuous variable (such as the age of the person) on a continuous variable (such as body mass), we’ll use an ANCOVA (an analysis of covariance). If we’re studying the effect of multiple independent factors, we’ll use the MANOVA instead of the ANOVA and the MANCOVA (multivariable analysis of variance and covariance) instead of the ANCOVA. If we’re simultaneously studying the effects of continuous and discrete variables (age, gender, size of residence) on binary variables (infected by Toxoplasma /uninfected by Toxoplasma), we’ll use a logistic regression, whereas for an ordinal variable we’ll use an ordinal logistic regression. If we’re studying the relationship of two continuous variables, we’ll use a correlation analysis, and if we also know or have decided beforehand which of the two variables is dependent (target) and which is explanatory, we’ll use a linear regression; if we’re studying the dependence of a continuous variable on multiple continuous variables, we’ll use a multidimensional linear regression. If we’re studying the relationship between two categorical variables, we’ll use contingency tables; if it’s between great numbers of categorical variables, we’ll use log-linear analysis. In all the above-mentioned cases we can also use the more universal GLM (General Linear Model) method, and, if needed, the even more universal GLZ (Generalized Linear Model method, often also donoted as GLM to make the orientation in the method more difficult for nonstatisticians). A nonparametric analogue of the common (unpaired) t-test is the Mann-Whitney U-test; an analogue of the paired t-test is the Wilcoxon test; an analogue of the simple ANOVA is the Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA; an approximate analogue of the two-way ANOVA is a Friedman test; and an analogue of correlation analysis is a Spearman’s rank correlation test, or a Kendall’s Tau test. Aside from hypotheses of the location (average, median value), we often test a hypothesis of the equality of the empirical and theoretical frequency distributions (most often using the Chi2-test); or we test a hypothesis of the equality of distribution of values in two or more sets (using the F-test); or a hypothesis of outlying values (using Grubbs’ or Dixon’s test).agreement with the statement: “I don’t have a strong sense of self-preservation: in situations, when others would probably be scared, at night alone in the woods or in a deserted house, I am not particularly afraid.” (P = 0.029).

We subsequently included the same questions, or at least some of them, in other questionnaire-based studies. We never again accomplished such strong associations with so many questions, yet even here the significant effect of toxoplasmosis on the subjects’ (blood donors, pregnant women) answers to some of these questions was apparent. Most often the test subjects mentioned that they have slower reflexes and that they believe that some people are able to impose their will on others using hypnosis or persuasion (17).

The statements which I set to our test subjects were relatively complex, each made up of multiple partial assertions. In the study which I carried out in the years 2007-2011, aside from the original ten complex statements, we also gave the students individual partial and specifying assertions. At the start of the 44-point questionnaire we presented the original ten statements, followed by 34 partial assertions, such as: “My instinctive (reflexive) behavior in situations of imminent danger is disproportionately slow or passive.” or “At night alone in a deserted house I’m not particularly afraid.” The students filled out the questionnaire on the computer, which recorded not only their answers, but also the time they needed to select each answer (this time on a 5-point scale). The final results of this more detailed study are not yet available.

I already mentioned that publishing a description of an unknown phenomenon studied using a nonstandard method doesn’t tend to be exactly easy. Nevertheless I succeeded that time, and, what’s more, right on the first try (17). Apparently my results were clear and convincing, and my argument bullet-proof enough, that neither the editor nor the reviewers were able to raise any substantial objections. But we cannot bar the possibility, that a certain role may have been played by the fact that the editor of this particular special journal number, and therefore also the person who selected the reviewers, was I.

 

 

Was this information useful for you?
The classical Darwinian theory of evolution can explain the evolution of adaptive traits only in asexual organisms. The frozen plasticity theory is much more general: It can also explain the origin and evolution of adaptive traits in both asexual and sexual organisms Read more